Understanding MindBrain

 
 
Random Article


 
Latest Posts
 

Neurons in the frontal lobe may be responsible for rational decision-making

 

 
Overview
 

 
Summary
 
 
 
 
 


 


Bottom Line

From physorg.com — You study the menu at a restaurant and decide to order the steak rather than the salmon. But when the waiter tells you about the lobster special, you decide lobster trumps steak. Without reconsidering the salmon, you place your order—all because of a trait called “transitivity.” “Transitivity is the hallmark of rational […]

0
Posted December 10, 2007 by thomasr

 
Full Article
 
 

From physorg.com — You study the menu at a restaurant and decide to order the steak rather than the salmon. But when the waiter tells you about the lobster special, you decide lobster trumps steak. Without reconsidering the salmon, you place your order—all because of a trait called “transitivity.”

“Transitivity is the hallmark of rational economic choice,” says Camillo Padoa-Schioppa, a postdoctoral researcher in HMS Professor of Neurobiology John Assad’s lab. According to transitivity, if you prefer A to B and B to C, then you ought to prefer A to C. Or, if you prefer lobster to steak, and steak to salmon, then you will prefer lobster to salmon.

Padoa-Schioppa is lead author on a paper that suggests this trait might be encoded at the level of individual neurons. The study, which appears online Dec. 9 in Nature Neuroscience, shows that some neurons in a part of the brain called the orbitofrontal cortex encode economic value in a “menu invariant” way. That is, the neurons respond the same to steak regardless if it’s offered against salmon or lobster.

“People make choices by assigning values to different options. If the values are menu invariant preferences will be transitive. The
activity of these neurons does not vary with the menu options, suggesting that these neurons could be responsible for
transitivity,” Padoa-Schioppa explains.

“This study provides a key insight into the biology of our frontal lobes and the neural circuits that underlie decision-making,” Assad
adds. “Despite the maxim, we in fact can compare apples to oranges, and we do it all the time. Camillo’s research sheds light on how we
make these types of choices.”

Frontal lobe damage has been linked to “choice deficits” such as
eating disorders, compulsive gambling and abnormal social behavior. For
example, in the first documented case of brain injury impacting
behavior, the infamous railroad construction foreman Phineas Gage became
unsociable after a tamping iron passed through his skull in 1848,
damaging his frontal lobes. This area of the brain has also been
implicated in drug abuse.

Labs are just beginning to probe normal decision-making at the level of
individual neurons, venturing into a new field called neuroeconomics.
Such research might eventually help to explain choice deficits
associated with frontal lobe functions.

The new study builds on an April 2006 Nature paper in which
Padoa-Schioppa and Assad identified neurons that encode the value
macaque monkeys assign to juice they choose independent of its type,
providing a common currency of comparison for the brain.

In that study, the scientists found that although monkeys generally
prefer grape juice to apple juice, sometimes they choose the latter, if
it is offered in large amounts. When presented with 3 units of apple
juice and 1 unit of grape juice, for example, a monkey might take the
grape juice only 50 percent of the time. This indicates that the value
of the grape juice is 3 times that of the apple juice. A particular
group of neurons in the orbitofrontal cortex fire at roughly the same
rate, regardless of the monkey’s decision because the animal values
both choices equally. These neurons also fire at the same rate if the
monkey chooses 6 units of apple juice or 2 units of grape juice. Thus,
these neurons encode the value the monkey receives in each trial.

Now, by adding a third juice to the mix, the team has tested whether
these neurons reflect transitivity. The three juices were offered to a
monkey in pairs dozens of times over the course of a session, the
quantity of each juice varying from trial to trial.

In general, monkeys preferred 1 unit of juice A to 1 unit of juice B, 1B
to 1C, and 1A to 1C. During each session, Padoa-Schioppa recorded the
activity of a handful of neurons in the orbitofrontal cortex, and he
discovered their firing rate did not depend on whether B was offered
against A or against C, indicating that these neurons respond in a menu
invariant way.

“The stability of these neurons could help to explain why we make
decisions that are consistent over the short term,” Padoa-Schioppa
says. “In our study, the neural circuit was not influenced by the
short-term behavioral context.”

Padoa-Schioppa is now examining the possibility that value-encoding
neurons may adapt to different value scales over longer periods of time.

Source: Harvard Medical School


thomasr

 


0 Comments



Be the first to comment!


Leave a Response