Understanding MindBrain

 
 
Random Article


 
Latest Posts
 

Seeing sounds, hearing tastes – Synesthesia in brain and mind

 

 
Overview
 

 
Summary
 
 
 
 
 


 


Bottom Line

Do all people experience stimulation of each sense independently? Accumulating evidence suggests that a special kind of perceptual phenomenon – syhesthesia – leads to a confusion of the specific senses. The problem is to relate these personal accounts to the science of the brain. In 1915, the Russian composer Alexander Scriabin had his “Prometheus” played […]

4
Posted August 24, 2002 by thomasr

 
Full Article
 
 

article_image.jpegDo all people experience stimulation of each sense independently? Accumulating evidence suggests that a special kind of perceptual phenomenon – syhesthesia – leads to a confusion of the specific senses. The problem is to relate these personal accounts to the science of the brain.
In 1915, the Russian composer Alexander Scriabin had his “Prometheus” played for the first time. The orchestral piece was originally written for orchestra, piano, pipe organ, choir and light organ — literally an organ that creates light. You may ask what a light organ is, and you would be perfectly right if you guessed that there is no such thing. So why did Scriabin compose a piece of music for a fictive instrument? The answer lies in the concept of “synesthesia”: the perceptual crossover between senses. Scriabin probably did not believe that one could ever play what he had composed for the light organ – he simply did not know of any other way to describe his conception of the music when the “Prometheus” was played. Although it is still debated whether Scriabin was a “true” synesthete, the Prometheus is often regarded as one of many examples of the phenomenon.

Synesthesia is the perceptual crossover between senses Many others have claimed to be so-called synesthetes, among them Wassily Kandinsky, Vladimir Nabokov, Richard Feynman and Nikola Tesla. Nabokov, for example, had a quite specific form of “coloured hearing”, where the sounds of each of the letters of the alphabet evoked specific hues. As a child, he also sometimes complained that the numbers and letters on his block were “wrong”. His mother – also a synesthete – understood and sympathized.

Synesthesia stems from the Greek syn-aisthesis (“together-perception”), and is used for terming the phenomenon where a person has involuntary physical experience of a cross-modal experience. This means that stimulating a given sense produces an experience in another sense modality. The most common example is the “coloured hearing” cases, where a person experience colours when listening to a particular sound. The synesthesias are often quite specific and stable, so that separate instruments might evoke different visual sensations, e.g. their hues and forms.

How many people experience synestesia? Initially, this is rather difficult to answer, since it depends on how one choose to define the phenomenon. On the one hand, one could use a broad definition, including weaker associations like coupling the vocal “a” to seeing a red colour. Using what might initially seem a conservative definition, estimates are still quite high, ranging among children from 40-50%, to 10-20% among adults. On the other hand, others claim that the phenomenon is much more rare, at a rate at only 1 in 25.000 people.

Unprecedented work has been done by Sean Day, who catalogued 19 different kinds of synesthesia based upon 175 case histories (see table).

Image

Theories on synesthesia

Despite different assessments on the prevalence of synesthesia, one fact is less disputed; the phenomenon is much more prevalent in children than adults. But why is this the case? Some speculate that it reflects a general cognitive development. The famous imagery researcher Alan Paivio has claimed that children process information mainly by means of iconic representations, while adults process information in an abstract manner – in the form of symbolic representations. Building further on this, Marks has stated that cognitive development come in three stages, from purely sensory representation of perceptual information, through a joint sensory/verbal representation, to pure verbal representation. According to Marks, synesthesia does not vanish with age, but merely looses its reflexive, sensory character, and is more and more expressed through language. Support for this thought has been found in studies showing that adults have weak cross-modal associations, but that they are weak or come to expression through verbal analogies and metaphors, and not as “living” images, as found in true synesthetes.

There are two general theories about synesthesia In general, there are two major lines of thought pertaining to Synesthesia. First, there is the theory of synesthetic metaphors, which claims that Synesthesia is the result of a person’s vivid imagination. In this view, people that claim to experience synesthesia take metaphors, like “I see what you are getting at” and “that colour is very loud”, too literally. In the same line of thought, the theory of linguistic synesthesia claims that synesthesia is generated through semantic processes and fashioned by time and cultural elements. One central thesis stemming from this view is the expectation of cultural differences. That is, if synesthesia is moulded from linguistic acts and cultural influences, any cultures should possibly reveal differences in the kinds of synesthesias that are expressed among individuals. Although scientific investigation in this matter has been rather sparse, it now seems that there are few cultural variations in synesthesia.
Cytowic’s theory

The other type of theory on synesthesia is often described as “more scientific”, and follows theories from physics and neurological disorders, as well as the study of effects of psychoactive drugs. One of the foremost contemporary writers on synesthesia, Richard E. Cytowic, has proposed a heory of its neural basis. Important to his work is his definition of the phenomenon, which is comprised of several pieces, for example that synesthesia is:

  • neither voluntary or controllable by the subject, or constant – it is usually triggered by some stimulus
  • “projected” – perceived to take place in the area immediately surrounding the subject
  • durable and generic” – associations between the senses will be constant over time and will also be relatively abstract

One of Cytowic’s surprising claims is that synesthesia is not a result of cortical activity. This is in direct opposition to theories of the brain basis for normal conscious sensation. In general, most such theories assume Cytowic: synesthesia is not a result of cortical activity not only a cortical substrate per se, such as the primary sensory modalities, but also argue for the necessary role of extensive processing in the frontal areas of the cortex. Contrary to this, Cytowic cites several pieces of evidence that synesthesia is accompanied by increased limbic activity – that is, activity in structures “below” the cortex, often seen as more primitive structures. At the same time, cortical activity is decreased.

The main reason for Cytowic’s claim is based on an opposition to what he identifies as the Western notion of a dichotomy between reason and emotion, and the resulting models where cortex is placed as being of higher order than the more “primitive” and “lower” areas, such as the limbic system. By citing Ommaya, a critic of current brain modeling, Cytowic claims that the corticocentric view of the brain ignores the fact that “we are irrational creatures by design, and that emotion, not reason, may play the decisive role both in how we think and act”. The relationship between cortex and the limbic structures are not one of hierarchy and doninance, but rather of complex reciprocal communication and interdependence. Thus, if we are to accept Cytowic’s theory of synesthesia, we also are forced to accept his notion of brain design and functioning.

Burt and Smith-Laittan’s theory

Cytowic’s theory has not gone unchallenged. Burt and Smith-Laittan from Cambridge University base their theory on a rather more traditional, modular brain theory. They suggest that, due to some genetic factors, synesthesia may arise from an abnormal differentiationbetween visual and auditory pathways. That is, during normal development, each brain area that pertains to a certain sense is specialized and hence differentiated from other senses. Not so with synesthetes, where the brain circuitries for, say, the visual and the auditory pathways are still significantly more “intermingled”, which in turn functions as the basis for the abnormal sensory integration. Another consequence from this view is that it would seem that infants all alike are experiencing Synesthesia.

Burt and Smith-Laittan: synesthetes have a less modular brain In terms of a theory for the brain basis for consciousness, Burt and Smith-Laittan’s theory poses that the senses are hardwired from birth into certain cortical (and thalamic) areas. In infancy, the brain consists of a multiplicity and ambundance of neurons and connections. During development of an individual, however, neurons specializes, creating modules, nodes and other functional units. Neurons that cannot adapt and make significant connections, die. So, over time it would seem that conscious perception within a given sense is more and more isolated from other senses, and that the integration of senses is a more effortful and time consuming process. For the synesthetes, however, it would seem that their brains are not that specialized and “sensory isolated”. For future research, it will be interesting to see whether there are further perceptual differences between normal sensation and Synesthesia.

Summary

Like so many brain phenomena, synesthesia seems like hard to believe — unless you are a synesthete. But decades of scientific studies have shown that it is real. A better understanding may shed light on normal conscious perception, and perhaps even on the minds of highly creative people like Scriabin. If many children are natural synesthetes, we may also learn more about the conscious world of childhood. Surprising conditions always reveal unexpected insights into the brain, its development, and the human condition.

– Bottom: Read a personal account of the phenomenon

Links

Lingua Franca (2001) volume 11, no. 6

PSYCHE symposium on Synesthesia (go down on page)

Selected References

  1. Cytowic, R.E. (1995). Synesthesia: phenomenology and neuropsychology. A review of current knowledge. PSYCHE, 2 (10)
  2. McGrath, M.M. (1998). Tangled Wires: Conceptualising neurological and cultural explanations of Synesthesia
  3. Marks, L.E. (1975). On coloured hearing Synesthesia: Cross-modal translations of sensory dimensions. Psychological Bulletin, 82, 303-331.
  4. Paivio, A. (1971). Imagery and verbal processes. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.
  5. Svartdal, F. & Fossland, B. (1988). Synesthesia. A review and discussion. Nordisk Psykologi, 40, 48-63.

thomasr

 


4 Comments


  1.  
    Elise

    I myself am a synesthete, and I find the subject rather interesting. More people definitely should learn about it!




  2.  

    One of the sentences in this article says that synesthesia is a sort of a “neurological disorder”. I believe it to be so. I also believe that something like Baars’s “Workspace” is a proper place for such competing neurons, but now in conflict with their input and consequently synesthetic disorder in the output is noticed by synesthetes and may be registered at labs.

    All information coming from the sesnses ‘DO’ meet in the same field where mind (conscious & sub/unconscious)tackle it all so amazingly in a normal head. However, in an abnormal one indistinctive/ wrong judgement/(disorder) including the synesthesia phenomenon may partially or wholly occur.




  3.  

    One more concluding para. to be added to the above comment (no. 2):
    One’s saliva may run in one’s mouth (leak/ just like a stream) when the word lemon is mentioned, or lemon itself is seen or even imagined. The saliva flow increases if one looks at a lemon being cut. What does this mean? the sense of taste + the sense of sight (in the area of imagination where all senses meet, affect each other or dominate/ encroach upon each other’s areas due to certain causes/effects/circumstances)= synesthesia.





Leave a Response